Bio
Lee Ross is a professor of psychology at Stanford University and co-founder of the Stanford Center on Conflict and Negotiation The author of three influential books, Human Inference and the Person and the Situation (both with Richard Nisbett) and, more recently The Wisest One in the Room (with Thomas Gilovich) and many highly cited papers, his research on attributional biases and shortcomings in human inference has exerted a major impact in social psychology and the field of human inference, judgment and decision-making. Among the phenomena he identified and has explored are the fundamental attribution error, the false consensus effect, reactive devaluation, the hostile media phenomenon, and the convictions of naïve realism. More recently he has ventured into more applied domains, exploring psychological barriers to dispute resolution (most notably the phenomenon of reactive devaluation) and participating in conflict resolution efforts in the Middle East and Northern Ireland. He has also taken part in efforts to deal with other applied topics including telemarketer fraud directed against the elderly, the behavior aspects of health care utilization and the problem of combating global warming. Ross was elected in 1994 to the American Academy of Arts and Sciences and in 2010 to the National Academy of Sciences. He has also received distinguished career awards from the American Psychological Society and the Society of Experimental Social Psychology.
Education: University of Toronto BA, 1965. Columbia University PhD, 1979 (where he earned his PhD with Stanley Schachter. Upon graduation in 1969, he joined Stanford faculty)
Administrative Appointments
-
co-Founder, Stanford Center on International conflict and negotiation (1978 - Present)
Program Affiliations
-
Public Policy
Professional Education
-
Ph.D., Columbia University, Social Psychology (1969)
Current Research and Scholarly Interests
Attributional processes and biases. Strategies and shortcomings in lay judgment and decision making. Basis of (and biases in) knowledge about self and others; egocentrism and "naive realism." Sources of interpersonal and intergroup conflict. Barriers to conflict resolution and techniques for overcoming such barriers.
2023-24 Courses
-
Independent Studies (3)
- Graduate Research
PSYCH 275 (Sum) - Reading and Special Work
PSYCH 194 (Sum) - Special Laboratory Projects
PSYCH 195 (Sum)
- Graduate Research
-
Prior Year Courses
2020-21 Courses
- Advanced Research
PSYCH 197 (Aut) - Classic and contemporary social psychology research
PSYCH 212 (Aut) - Public Policy and Social Psychology: Implications and Applications
INTLPOL 207B, PSYCH 216, PUBLPOL 305B (Win) - Senior Honors Research
PSYCH 198 (Win, Spr)
- Advanced Research
All Publications
-
Intergroup Sentiments, Political Identity, and Their Influence on Responses to Potentially Ameliorative Proposals in the Context of an Intractable Conflict
JOURNAL OF CONFLICT RESOLUTION
2016; 60 (1): 61-88
View details for DOI 10.1177/0022002714535250
View details for Web of Science ID 000368178600003
-
The Climate Change Challenge and Barriers to the Exercise of Foresight Intelligence
BioScience
2016: 363-370
View details for DOI 10.1093/biosci/biw025
- The Mote in Thy Brother’s Eye Skeptical Inquirer 2016; 40.2: 40-44
- The letter to a friend that helped launch a career Scientists Making a Difference Cambridge University Press. 2016: 306–310
- Overcoming Relational Barriers to Agreement Negotiating in Times of Conflict 2015: 205–212
-
What kinds of conservatives does social psychology lack, and why?
Behavioral and Brain Sciences
2015; 38
View details for DOI 10.1017/S0140525X14001368
- The Bias Blind Spot and its Implications Contemporary Organizational Behavior: From Ideas to Action Pearson Prentice Hall. 2015: 137–145
- The WISEST ONE in the ROOM: HOW YOU CAN BENEFIT FROM SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY'S MOST POWERFUL INSIGHTS Free Press. 2015
-
Research-Based Knowledge in Psychology: What, if Anything, is Its Incremental Value to the Practitioner?
INTEGRATIVE PSYCHOLOGICAL AND BEHAVIORAL SCIENCE
2014; 48 (4): 365-383
View details for DOI 10.1007/s12124-014-9275-1
View details for Web of Science ID 000344390100001
-
Barriers to agreement in the asymmetric Israeli–Palestinian conflict
Dynamics of Asymmetric Conflict
2014; 48 (4): 120-136
View details for DOI 10.1080/17467586.2014.970565
-
Research-Based Knowledge in Psychology: What, if Anything, is Its Incremental Value to the Practitioner?
Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science
2014; 48 (4): 365–383
View details for DOI 10.1007/s12124-014-9275-1
-
Forewarning Reduces Fraud Susceptibility in Vulnerable Consumers
BASIC AND APPLIED SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
2014; 36 (3): 272-279
Abstract
Telemarketing fraud is pervasive and older consumers are disproportionally targeted. Given laboratory research showing that forewarning can effectively counter influence appeals, we conducted a field experiment to test whether forewarning could protect people who had been victimized in the past. A research assistant with prior experience as a telemarketer pitched a mock scam two or four weeks after participants were warned about the same scam or an entirely different scam. Both warnings reduced unequivocal acceptance of the mock scam although outright refusals (as opposed to expressions of skepticism) were more frequent with the same scam warning than the different scam warning. The same scam warning, but not the different scam warning, lost effectiveness over time. Findings demonstrate that social psychological research can inform effective protection strategies against telemarketing fraud.
View details for DOI 10.1080/01973533.2014.903844
View details for Web of Science ID 000337596500008
View details for PubMedCentralID PMC4199235
-
David L. Rosenhan (1929-2012).
American psychologist
2013; 68 (6): 469-?
Abstract
Presents an obituary for David L. Rosenhan (1929-2012). A distinguished psychologist and professor emeritus at Stanford University, Rosenhan died February 6, 2012, at the age of 82, after a long illness. Born in Jersey City, New Jersey, on November 22, 1929, he received a bachelor's degree in mathematics (1951) from Yeshiva College and a master's degree in economics (1953) and a doctorate in psychology (1958) from Columbia University. A professor of law and of psychology at Stanford University from 1971 until his retirement in 1998, Rosenhan was a pioneer in applying psychological methods to the practice of law, including the examination of expert witnesses, jury selection, and jury deliberation. A former president of the American Psychology-Law Society and of the American Board of Forensic Psychology, Rosenhan was a fellow of the American Association for the Advancement of Science, of the American Psychological Association, and of the American Psychological Society. Before joining the Stanford Law School faculty, he was a member of the faculties of Swarthmore College, Princeton University, Haverford College, and the University of Pennsylvania. He also served as a research psychologist at the Educational Testing Service. As generations of Stanford students can attest, David Rosenhan was a spellbinding lecturer who managed to convey the sense that he was speaking to each individual, no matter how large the group. To his graduate students, he was consistently encouraging and optimistic, always ready to share a joke or story, and gently encouraging of their creativity and progressive independence as researchers. The lessons he cared most about offering, in the classroom as in his research, were about human dignity and the need to confront abuse of power and human frailties. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2013 APA, all rights reserved).
View details for DOI 10.1037/a0032245
View details for PubMedID 24016118
-
The meaning of default options for potential organ donors
PROCEEDINGS OF THE NATIONAL ACADEMY OF SCIENCES OF THE UNITED STATES OF AMERICA
2012; 109 (38): 15201-15205
Abstract
Rates of participation in organ donation programs are known to be powerfully influenced by the relevant default policy in effect ("opt-in" vs. "opt-out"). Three studies provide evidence that this difference in participation may occur in part because the requirement to opt-in or opt-out results in large differences in the meaning that individuals attach to participation. American participants in Study 1 rated participation as a significantly more substantial action when agreement was purportedly obtained under opt-in rather than opt-out conditions, and nonagreement as a greater abrogation of responsibility when that decision was made under opt-out rather than under opt-in conditions. Study 2 replicated these findings with respondents who live in Germany, which employs an opt-in donation policy, and in Austria, which has an opt-out policy. Study 3 required American participants to rate various actions that differ in the effort and self-sacrifice they demand. As predicted, the placement of organ donation on the resulting multidimensional scaling dimension differed significantly depending on whether it purportedly was made in an opt-in country (where it was considered roughly akin to giving away half of one's wealth to charity upon one's death) or an opt-out country (where it fell between letting others get ahead of one in line and volunteering some time to help the poor). We discuss the relationship between this change of meaning account and two other mechanisms-behavioral inertia and implicit norms-that we believe underlie the default effect in decision making and other effects of policies designed to influence decision-makers.
View details for DOI 10.1073/pnas.1211695109
View details for Web of Science ID 000309211000034
View details for PubMedID 22949639
-
Albert H. Hastorf III (1921-2011)
AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGIST
2012; 67 (6): 493-493
Abstract
Presents an obituary for Albert H. Hastorf III. Albert H. Hastorf III, a pioneer in the study of social perception and interaction and a celebrated member of the Stanford University administration, died September 26, 2011, in Palo Alto, California. Al was known early in his career as the coauthor of one of social psychology's most famous studies-a study that vividly illustrated the constructive and potentially biased nature of perception-and his contributions to psychology and American academia were wide-ranging. Hastorf joined Stanford's faculty in 1961, serving as executive head of the Psychology Department from 1961 to 1970. He was also a founder of the university's Interdisciplinary Human Biology Program, soon one of Stanford's most popular majors and an attractive gateway for students interested in medicine. Al's unique gifts as an administrator were apparent to all who knew him. His sound judgment, personal graciousness, good humor, and unquestioned integrity made him a popular choice as dean of the School of Humanities & Sciences from 1970 to 1974 and as provost from 1980 to 1984. The esteem in which Al was held by the Stanford community was recognized with a succession of awards, including the Lloyd W. Dinkelspiel Award for Outstanding Service to Undergraduate Education and the Richard W. Lyman Award for unique and dedicated service to the university.
View details for DOI 10.1037/a0029596
View details for Web of Science ID 000309300900007
View details for PubMedID 22963416
-
How Christians reconcile their personal political views and the teachings of their faith: Projection as a means of dissonance reduction
PROCEEDINGS OF THE NATIONAL ACADEMY OF SCIENCES OF THE UNITED STATES OF AMERICA
2012; 109 (10): 3616-3622
Abstract
The present study explores the dramatic projection of one's own views onto those of Jesus among conservative and liberal American Christians. In a large-scale survey, the relevant views that each group attributed to a contemporary Jesus differed almost as much as their own views. Despite such dissonance-reducing projection, however, conservatives acknowledged the relevant discrepancy with regard to "fellowship" issues (e.g., taxation to reduce economic inequality and treatment of immigrants) and liberals acknowledged the relevant discrepancy with regard to "morality" issues (e.g., abortion and gay marriage). However, conservatives also claimed that a contemporary Jesus would be even more conservative than themselves on the former issues whereas liberals claimed that Jesus would be even more liberal than themselves on the latter issues. Further reducing potential dissonance, liberal and conservative Christians differed markedly in the types of issues they claimed to be more central to their faith. A concluding discussion considers the relationship between individual motivational processes and more social processes that may underlie the present findings, as well as implications for contemporary social and political conflict.
View details for DOI 10.1073/pnas.1117557109
View details for Web of Science ID 000301117700012
View details for PubMedID 22308413
View details for PubMedCentralID PMC3309730
-
Naive realism and capturing the "wisdom of dyads"
JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
2012; 48 (2): 507-512
View details for DOI 10.1016/j.jesp.2011.10.016
View details for Web of Science ID 000301685800008
-
REFLECTIONS ON BIASED ASSIMILATION AND BELIEF POLARIZATION
CRITICAL REVIEW
2012; 24 (2): 233-245
View details for DOI 10.1080/08913811.2012.711025
View details for Web of Science ID 000309119800006
- Perspectives on disagreement and dispute resolution: Lessons from the lab and the real world The Behavioral Foundations of Public Policy Princeton University & Russell Sage Foundation Press. 2012: 108–125
-
Two Social Psychologists' Reflections on Situationism and the Criminal Justice System
Ideology, Psychology, and Law Ideology, Psychology, and Law
Oxford University Press. 2012
View details for DOI 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199737512.003.0024
- Barriers to Dispute Resolution: Reflections on Peacemaking and Relationships between Adversaries Understanding social action, promoting human rights Oxford University Press. 2012: 265–291
-
Two to Tango: Effects of Collaboration and Disagreement on Dyadic Judgment
PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN
2011; 37 (10): 1325-1338
Abstract
Four studies examined dyadic collaboration on quantitative estimation tasks. In accord with the tenets of "naïve realism," dyad members failed to give due weight to a partner's estimates, especially those greatly divergent from their own. The requirement to reach joint estimates through discussion increased accuracy more than reaching agreement through a mere exchange of numerical "bids." However, even the latter procedure increased accuracy, relative to that of individual estimates (Study 1). Accuracy feedback neither increased weight given to partner's subsequent estimates nor produced improved accuracy (Study 2). Long-term dance partners, who shared a positive estimation bias, failed to improve accuracy when estimating their performance scores (Study 3). Having dyad members ask questions about the bases of partner's estimates produced greater yielding and accuracy increases than having them explain their own estimates (Study 4). The latter two studies provided additional direct and indirect evidence for the role of naïve realism.
View details for DOI 10.1177/0146167211410436
View details for Web of Science ID 000294473900004
View details for PubMedID 21632960
-
Affirming the Self to Promote Agreement With Another: Lowering a Psychological Barrier to Conflict Resolution
PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN
2011; 37 (9): 1216-1228
Abstract
Two studies investigated the capacity of a self-affirmation intervention to lower a psychological barrier to conflict resolution. Study 1 used a role-play scenario in which a student negotiated with a professor for greater rewards for work on a collaborative project. A self-affirmation manipulation, in which participants focused on an important personal value, significantly reduced their tendency to derogate a concession offered by the professor relative to one that had not been offered. Study 2 replicated this effect and showed that the phenomenon did not depend on the self-affirmed participant's experience of a heightened sense of deservingness or a tendency to make positive attributions about the professor. Distraction and explicit mood enhancement were also ruled out as mediators of the self-affirmation effect, which appears to stem from motivational rather than explicit cognitive processes.
View details for DOI 10.1177/0146167211409439
View details for Web of Science ID 000293081600006
View details for PubMedID 21586689
-
Affirmation, Acknowledgment of In-Group Responsibility, Group-Based Guilt, and Support for Reparative Measures
JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
2011; 101 (2): 256-270
Abstract
Three studies, 2 conducted in Israel and 1 conducted in Bosnia and Herzegovina, demonstrated that affirming a positive aspect of the self can increase one's willingness to acknowledge in-group responsibility for wrongdoing against others, express feelings of group-based guilt, and consequently provide greater support for reparation policies. By contrast, affirming one's group, although similarly boosting feelings of pride, failed to increase willingness to acknowledge and redress in-group wrongdoing. Studies 2 and 3 demonstrated the mediating role of group-based guilt. That is, increased acknowledgment of in-group responsibility for out-group victimization produced increased feelings of guilt, which in turn increased support for reparation policies to the victimized group. Theoretical and applied implications are discussed.
View details for DOI 10.1037/a0023936
View details for Web of Science ID 000292846600004
View details for PubMedID 21639648
-
Wishful Thinking: Belief, Desire, and the Motivated Evaluation of Scientific Evidence
PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE
2011; 22 (6): 731-732
View details for DOI 10.1177/0956797611406447
View details for Web of Science ID 000294709200005
View details for PubMedID 21515736
-
Achieving difficult agreements: Effects of Positive Expectations on negotiation processes and outcomes
JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
2010; 46 (3): 494-504
View details for DOI 10.1016/j.jesp.2009.12.010
View details for Web of Science ID 000277218000002
-
A history of social psychology: Insights, contributions, and challenges
The Handbook of Social Psychology
Random House. 2010
View details for DOI 10.1002/9780470561119.socpsy001001
-
Happiness and Memory: Affective Significance of Endowment and Contrast
EMOTION
2009; 9 (5): 666-680
Abstract
Three studies (two conducted in Israel and one in the United States) examined associations between self-rated dispositional happiness and tendencies to treat memories of positive and negative events as sources of enhanced or attenuated happiness through the use of "endowment" and "contrast." Although participants generally endorsed items describing happiness-enhancing tendencies more than happiness-diminishing ones, self-reported happiness was associated with greater endorsement of "positive endowment" items and less endorsement of "negative endowment" items, and also with less endorsement of items that involved contrasting the present with happier times in the past. Only in the American sample, however, was happiness associated with greater endorsement of items that involved contrasting the present with less happy times in the past. These data suggest that relatively unhappy people show somewhat conflicting memorial tendencies vis-à-vis happiness, whereas very happy people show simpler, and less conflicting, tendencies. These findings augment the existing literatures on the affective consequences of memory, which have been concerned more with mood than with temperament and/or have dealt only with a subset of the endowment and contrast tendencies explored in the present work.
View details for DOI 10.1037/a0016816
View details for Web of Science ID 000270700100008
View details for PubMedID 19803589
-
Political mindset: Effects of schema priming on liberal-conservative political positions
JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
2009; 45 (4): 890-895
View details for DOI 10.1016/j.jesp.2009.04.007
View details for Web of Science ID 000269278800033
- Dealing with Conflict: Experiences and Experiments The Scientist and the Humanist: A Festschrift in Honor of Elliot Aronson Psychology Press. 2009: 39–66
-
Relational accommodation in negotiation: Effects of egalitarianism and gender on economic efficiency and relational capital
ORGANIZATIONAL BEHAVIOR AND HUMAN DECISION PROCESSES
2008; 107 (2): 192-205
View details for DOI 10.1016/j.obhdp.2008.02.009
View details for Web of Science ID 000261293200007
-
Acknowledging the other side in negotiation
NEGOTIATION JOURNAL
2008; 24 (3): 269-285
View details for DOI 10.1111/j.1571-9979.2008.00184.x
View details for Web of Science ID 000257497700004
- Environmental Values and Behaviors: Strategies to Encourage Public Support for Initiatives to Combat Global Warming Virginia Environmental Law Journal 2008; 26: 161-187
-
Bridging the partisan divide: Self-affirmation reduces ideological closed-mindedness and inflexibility in negotiation
JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
2007; 93 (3): 415-430
Abstract
Three studies link resistance to probative information and intransigence in negotiation to concerns of identity maintenance. Each shows that affirmations of personal integrity (vs. nonaffirmation or threat) can reduce resistance and intransigence but that this effect occurs only when individuals' partisan identity and/or identity-related convictions are made salient. Affirmation made participants' assessment of a report critical of U.S. foreign policy less dependent on their political views, but only when the identity relevance of the issue rather than the goal of rationality was salient (Study 1). Affirmation increased concession making in a negotiation over abortion policy, but again this effect was moderated by identity salience (Studies 2 and 3). Indeed, although affirmed negotiators proved relatively more open to compromise when either the salience of their true convictions or the importance of remaining faithful to those convictions was heightened, the reverse was true when the salient goal was compromise. The theoretical and applied significance of these findings are discussed.
View details for DOI 10.1037/0022-3514.93.3.415
View details for Web of Science ID 000248992100007
View details for PubMedID 17723057
-
Race, Crime and Antidiscrimination
Beyond Common Sense: Psychological Science in the Courtroom
Blackwell. 2007: 3–22
View details for DOI 10.1002/9780470696422.ch1
-
Idiosyncratic matching and choice: When less is more
ORGANIZATIONAL BEHAVIOR AND HUMAN DECISION PROCESSES
2006; 101 (2): 168-183
View details for DOI 10.1016/j.obhdp.2006.01.006
View details for Web of Science ID 000242818300003
-
Discrimination and implicit bias in a racially unequal society
CALIFORNIA LAW REVIEW
2006; 94 (4): 1169-1190
View details for Web of Science ID 000240300400006
-
Temporal differences in trait self-ascription: When the self is seen as an other
JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
2006; 90 (2): 197-209
Abstract
Seven studies exploring people's tendency to make observer-like attributions about their past and future selves are presented. Studies 1 and 2 showed temporal differences in trait assessments that paralleled the classic actor-observer difference. Study 3 provided evidence against a motivational account of these differences. Studies 4-7 explored underlying mechanisms involving differences in the focus of attention of the sort linked to the classic actor-observer difference. In Study 4, people perceived past and future selves from a more observer-like perspective than present selves. In Studies 5 and 6, manipulating attention to internal states (vs. observable behavior) of past and future selves led people to ascribe fewer traits to those selves. Study 7 showed an inverse relationship for past and present selves between observer-like visual focus and salience of internal information.
View details for DOI 10.1037/0022-3514.90.2.197
View details for Web of Science ID 000236445600001
View details for PubMedID 16536646
-
Peering into the bias blind spot: People's assessments of bias in themselves and others
PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN
2005; 31 (5): 680-692
Abstract
People tend to believe that their own judgments are less prone to bias than those of others, in part because they tend to rely on introspection for evidence of bias in themselves but on their lay theories in assessing bias in others. Two empirical consequences of this asymmetry are explored. Studies 1 and 2 document that people are more inclined to think they are guilty of bias in the abstract than in any specific instance. Studies 3 and 4 demonstrate that people tend to believe that their own personal connection to a given issue is a source of accuracy and enlightenment but that such personal connections in the case of others who hold different views are a source of bias. The implications of this asymmetry in assessing objectivity and bias in the self versus others are discussed.
View details for DOI 10.1177/0146167204271570
View details for Web of Science ID 000228472900008
View details for PubMedID 15802662
-
Psychological dimensions of the Israeli settlements issue: Endowments and identities
Conference on Past, Present and Future of the Jewish West Bank and Gaza Settlements - The Internal Israeli Conflict
WILEY-BLACKWELL PUBLISHING, INC. 2005: 209–19
View details for DOI 10.1111/j.0748-4526.2005.00058.x
View details for Web of Science ID 000227836500006
-
Material priming: The influence of mundane physical objects on situational construal and competitive behavioral choice
ORGANIZATIONAL BEHAVIOR AND HUMAN DECISION PROCESSES
2004; 95 (1): 83-96
View details for DOI 10.1016/j.obhdp.2004.06.003
View details for Web of Science ID 000223901300005
-
The name of the game: Predictive power of reputations versus situational labels in determining prisoner's dilemma game moves
PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN
2004; 30 (9): 1175-1185
Abstract
Two experiments, one conducted with American college students and one with Israeli pilots and their instructors, explored the predictive power of reputation-based assessments versus the stated "name of the game" (Wall Street Game vs. Community Game) in determining players' responses in an N-move Prisoner's Dilemma. The results of these studies showed that the relevant labeling manipulations exerted far greater impact on the players' choice to cooperate versus defect--both in the first round and overall--than anticipated by the individuals who had predicted their behavior. Reputation-based prediction, by contrast, failed to discriminate cooperators from defectors. A supplementary questionnaire study showed the generality of the relevant short-coming in naïve psychology. The implications of these findings, and the potential contribution of the present methodology to the classic pedagogical strategy of the demonstration experiment, are discussed.
View details for DOI 10.1177/0146167204264004
View details for Web of Science ID 000223215500007
View details for PubMedID 15359020
-
Objectivity in the eye of the beholder: Divergent perceptions of bias in self versus others
PSYCHOLOGICAL REVIEW
2004; 111 (3): 781-799
Abstract
Important asymmetries between self-perception and social perception arise from the simple fact that other people's actions, judgments, and priorities sometimes differ from one's own. This leads people not only to make more dispositional inferences about others than about themselves (E. E. Jones & R. E. Nisbett, 1972) but also to see others as more susceptible to a host of cognitive and motivational biases. Although this blind spot regarding one's own biases may serve familiar self-enhancement motives, it is also a product of the phenomenological stance of naive realism. It is exacerbated, furthermore, by people's tendency to attach greater credence to their own introspections about potential influences on judgment and behavior than they attach to similar introspections by others. The authors review evidence, new and old, of this asymmetry and its underlying causes and discuss its relation to other psychological phenomena and to interpersonal and intergroup conflict.
View details for DOI 10.1037/0033-295X.111.3.781
View details for Web of Science ID 000222380200008
View details for PubMedID 15250784
-
Dynamic valuation: Preference changes in the context of face-to-face negotiation
JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
2004; 40 (2): 142-151
View details for DOI 10.1016/j.jesp.2003.12.002
View details for Web of Science ID 000220023000002
-
Identity bifurcation in response to stereotype threat: Women and mathematics
JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
2004; 40 (2): 152-168
View details for DOI 10.1016/S0022-1031(03)00088-X
View details for Web of Science ID 000220023000003
-
Naive realism and affirmative action: Adversaries are more similar than they think
BASIC AND APPLIED SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
2003; 25 (4): 275-289
View details for Web of Science ID 000186852700002
-
The perceptual push: The interplay of implicit cues and explicit situational construals on behavioral intentions in the Prisoner's Dilemma
JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
2003; 39 (6): 634-643
View details for DOI 10.1016/S0022-1031(03)00057-X
View details for Web of Science ID 000186403000011
-
Contemporary psychology's challenges to legal theory and practice
NORTHWESTERN UNIVERSITY LAW REVIEW
2003; 97 (3): 1081-1114
View details for Web of Science ID 000183052200003
-
Culture and morality: Conflicting perspectives at a time of cultural confrontation - Essay review of the culture of morality: Social development, context, and conflict by Elliot Turiel
HUMAN DEVELOPMENT
2003; 46 (2-3): 151-154
View details for DOI 10.1159/000068587
View details for Web of Science ID 000181694600008
-
Reactive devaluation of an "Israeli" vs. "Palestinian" peace proposal
JOURNAL OF CONFLICT RESOLUTION
2002; 46 (4): 515-546
View details for Web of Science ID 000176766300003
-
The bias blind spot: Perceptions of bias in self versus others
PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN
2002; 28 (3): 369-381
View details for Web of Science ID 000173887300008
- Understanding Misunderstanding: Social Psychological Perspectives Heuristics and Biases; The Psychology of Intuitive Judgment Cambridge University Press. 2002: 636–665
-
You don't know me, but I know you: The illusion of asymmetric insight
106th Annual Convention of the American-Psychological-Association
AMER PSYCHOLOGICAL ASSOC. 2001: 639–56
Abstract
People, it is hypothesized, show an asymmetry in assessing their own interpersonal and intrapersonal knowledge relative to that of their peers. Six studies suggested that people perceive their knowledge of their peers to surpass their peers' knowledge of them. Several of the studies explored sources of this perceived asymmetry, especially the conviction that while observable behaviors (e.g., interpersonal revelations or idiosyncratic word completions) are more revealing of others than self, private thoughts and feelings are more revealing of self than others. Study 2 also found that college roommates believe they know themselves better than their peers know themselves. Study 6 showed that group members display a similar bias-they believe their groups know and understand relevant out-groups better than vice versa. The relevance of such illusions of asymmetric insight for interpersonal interaction and our understanding of "naive realism" is discussed.
View details for DOI 10.1037//0022-3514.81.4.639
View details for Web of Science ID 000171577400007
View details for PubMedID 11642351
-
Getting down to fundamentals: Lay dispositionism and the attributions of psychologists
PSYCHOLOGICAL INQUIRY
2001; 12 (1): 37-40
View details for Web of Science ID 000167627800009
-
Naive realism in everyday life and its implications for the misunderstanding
PSIKHOLOGICHESKII ZHURNAL
2000; 21 (6): 24-37
View details for Web of Science ID 000167353400003
-
The mentor's dilemma: Providing critical feedback across the racial divide
PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN
1999; 25 (10): 1302-1318
View details for Web of Science ID 000082661200011
-
Changes in attractiveness of elected, rejected, and precluded alternatives: A comparison of happy and unhappy individuals
JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
1999; 76 (6): 988-1007
Abstract
In 3 studies the authors compared the responses of self-rated happy and unhappy students in situations involving choice. In Study 1, high school seniors evaluated colleges after applying for admission and then later after making their selections. Happy students tended to be more satisfied than unhappy ones with the colleges they ultimately chose and those they ultimately rejected, and they more sharply devalued the colleges that rejected them. Studies 2 and 3 dealt with postdecisional consequences of less consequential decisions about fancy desserts. In Study 2, unhappy participants sharply derogated the desserts they rejected or were denied, relative to those selected by or for them, whereas happy participants showed no such derogation. These group differences, moreover, proved to be largely independent of self-esteem and optimism. The design of Study 3 helped explicate underlying mechanisms by inducing both groups to distract themselves or to self-reflect. Doing so eliminated all group differences. Implications of the results for the link between cognitive processes and hedonic consequences are discussed.
View details for Web of Science ID 000081061600008
View details for PubMedID 10402682
- Fairness norms and the potential for mutual agreements involving majority and minority groups. Research on managing groups and teams/Volume 2 JAI Press. 1999: 93–114
-
Naive realism in everyday life: Implications for the dynamics of social conflict
VOPROSY PSIKHOLOGII
1999: 61-?
View details for Web of Science ID 000084181700007
- Independence from whom? Interdependence with whom? Cultural perspectives on ingroups versus outgroups Cultural divides Sage. 1999: 273–301
- Framing Effects and Income Flow Preferences in Decisions about Social Security Behavioral Dimensions of Retirement Economics Brookings Institution Press. 1999: 187–209
-
The role of stereotyping in overconfident social prediction
SOCIAL COGNITION
1998; 16 (2): 225-252
View details for Web of Science ID 000075549900002
-
Comment on Gilbert
Conference in honor of Edward Ellsworth Jones
AMER PSYCHOLOGICAL ASSOC. 1998: 53–66
View details for Web of Science ID 000078348600004
-
Hedonic consequences of social comparison: A contrast of happy and unhappy people
JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
1997; 73 (6): 1141-1157
Abstract
Two studies tested the hypothesis that self-rated unhappy individuals would be more sensitive to social comparison information than would happy ones. Study 1 showed that whereas unhappy students' affect and self-assessments were heavily affected by a peer who solved anagrams either faster or slower, happy students' responses were affected by the presence of a slower peer only. These between-group differences proved to be largely independent of 2 factors associated with happiness, i.e., self-esteem and optimism. Study 2 showed that whereas the unhappy group's responses to feedback about their own teaching performance were heavily influenced by a peer who performed even better or even worse, happy students' responses again were moderated only by information about inferior peer performance. Implications for our appreciation of the link between cognitive processes and "hedonic" consequences are discussed.
View details for Web of Science ID A1997YK98900001
View details for PubMedID 9418274
-
Self-interest and fairness in problems of resource allocation: Allocators versus recipients
JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
1997; 72 (5): 1061-1074
Abstract
Two studies explored the tension between self-interest and the equality norm in problems of resource allocation. Study 1 presented graduate business students with a hypothetical task requiring them to make a series of managerial decisions. On learning the outcome of those decisions, they were asked to divide a bonus pool between self and a rival manager (who had opted for very different decisions and achieved either the same results as self on 2 criteria or a better result on 1 criterion and a worse result on the other criterion). Study 2 required Stanford and San Jose State undergraduates to consider the division of a hypothetical scholarship fund between candidates from their 2 schools. Data from both studies contrasted the apparent evenhandedness and lack of self-interest manifested by allocators with the self-serving responses of evaluators. Furthermore, when faced with different claims, participants were inclined to justify an unequal allocation of resources--provided that they, or a representative of their group, received the larger share--that few personally would have recommended, demanded, or imposed.
View details for Web of Science ID A1997WX85600008
View details for PubMedID 9150585
-
Naive realism in everyday life: Implications for social conflict and misunderstanding
23rd Annual Symposium of the Jean-Piaget-Society
LAWRENCE ERLBAUM ASSOC PUBL. 1996: 103–135
View details for Web of Science ID A1996BF35M00006
-
ACTUAL VERSUS ASSUMED DIFFERENCES IN CONSTRUAL - NAIVE REALISM IN INTERGROUP PERCEPTION AND CONFLICT
JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
1995; 68 (3): 404-417
View details for Web of Science ID A1995QK44900004
- Strategic, psychological, and institutional barriers: An introduction. Barriers to conflict resolution Norton. 1995: 2–25
- Barriers to conflict resolution edited by Arrow, K., Mnookin, R., Ross, L., Tversky, A., Wilson, R. W.W. Norton. 1995
-
Psychological Barriers to Dispute Resolution
Advances in Experimental Social Psychology
1995; 27: 255-304
View details for DOI 10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60407-4
- The reactive devaluation in negotiation & conflict resolution Barriers to conflict resolution Norton. 1995: 26–42
- Psychological barriers to conflict resolution. The Arab-Israeli negotiations: Political positions and conceptual frameworks Papyrus. 1993
- Perspectives on Personality and Social Psychology: Books waiting to be written Psychological Inquiry 1992; 3(1): 99-102
-
BARRIERS TO CONFLICT-RESOLUTION
NEGOTIATION JOURNAL-ON THE PROCESS OF DISPUTE SETTLEMENT
1991; 7 (4): 389-404
View details for Web of Science ID A1991GP18400005
-
SUBJECTIVE CONSTRUAL, SOCIAL INFERENCE, AND HUMAN MISUNDERSTANDING
ADVANCES IN EXPERIMENTAL SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
1991; 24: 319-359
View details for Web of Science ID A1991GN46100007
- The person and the situation : perspectives of social psychology McGraw-Hill series in social psychology McGraw Hill. 1991
-
THE ROLE OF CONSTRUAL PROCESSES IN OVERCONFIDENT PREDICTIONS ABOUT THE SELF AND OTHERS
JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
1990; 59 (6): 1128-1139
Abstract
Overconfident behavioral predictions and trait inferences may occur because people make inadequate allowance for the uncertainties of situational construal. In Studies 1-3, Ss estimated how much time or money they would spend in various hypothetical, incompletely specified situations. Ss then offered associated "confidence limits" under different "construal conditions". In Study 4, Ss made trait inferences about someone they believed had responded "deviantly"--again with situational details unspecified and construal conditions manipulated. In all 4 studies, Ss who made predictions or trait inferences without being able to assume the accuracy of their situational construals offered confidence limits no broader than those of Ss who made their responses contingent on such accuracy. Only in conditions where Ss were obliged to offer alternative construals did they appropriately broaden their confidence limits or weaken their trait inferences.
View details for Web of Science ID A1990EM79900005
View details for PubMedID 2283587
-
OVERCONFIDENT PREDICTION OF FUTURE ACTIONS AND OUTCOMES BY SELF AND OTHERS
JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
1990; 58 (4): 582-592
Abstract
In a follow-up study to Dunning, Griffin, Milojkovic, and L. Ross (1990), which had investigated the phenomenon of overconfidence in social prediction, two samples of first-year undergraduates were invited to make predictions about their own future responses (and, in the case of Sample 2, also those of their roommates) over the months ahead. These predictions were accompanied by confidence estimates and were evaluated in the light of actual responses reported later by the subjects in question. The primary finding was that self-predictions, like social predictions, proved to be consistently overconfident. As in Dunning et al., moreover, overconfidence could be traced to two sources. First, expressions of particularly high confidence rarely proved to be warranted; as confidence increased, the gap between accuracy and confidence widened. Second, predictions that went against relevant base rates yielded very low accuracy in the face of relatively unattenuated confidence levels. The implications of these results are discussed, and one potentially important underlying mechanism--the failure to make adequate inferential allowance for the uncertainties of situational construal--is proposed for further research.
View details for Web of Science ID A1990DC44300003
View details for PubMedID 2348360
-
THE OVERCONFIDENCE EFFECT IN SOCIAL PREDICTION
JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
1990; 58 (4): 568-581
Abstract
In five studies with overlapping designs and intents, subjects predicted a specific peer's responses to a variety of stimulus situations, each of which offered a pair of mutually exclusive and exhaustive response alternatives. Each prediction was accompanied by a subjective probability estimate reflecting the subjects' confidence in its accuracy--a measure validated in Study 5 by having subjects choose whether to "gamble" on the accuracy of their prediction or on the outcome of a simple aleatory event. Our primary finding was that in social prediction, as in other judgmental domains, subjects consistently proved to be highly overconfident. That is, regardless of the type of prediction item (e.g., responses to hypothetical dilemmas, responses to contrived laboratory situations, or self-reports of everyday behaviors) and regardless of the type of information available about the person whose responses they were predicting (e.g., predictions about roommates or predictions based on prior interviews), the levels of accuracy subjects achieved fell considerably below the levels required to justify their confidence levels. Further analysis revealed two specific sources of overconfidence. First, subjects generally were overconfident to the extent they were highly confident. Second, subjects were most likely to be overconfident when they knowingly or unknowingly made predictions that ran counter to the relevant response base rates and, as a consequence, achieved low accuracy rates that their confidence estimates failed to anticipate. Theoretical and normative implications are discussed and proposals for subsequent research offered.
View details for Web of Science ID A1990DC44300002
View details for PubMedID 2348359
- Recognizing the role of construal processes. The legacy of Solomon Asch: Essays in cognition and social psychology. edited by Rock, I. Erlbaum. 1990: 77–96
- Lee Ross, Personal Narrative A narrative history of experimental social psychology: The Lewin Tradition edited by Patnoe, S. Springer Verlag. 1988: 151–168
-
Situationist Perspectives on the obedience experiments. Review of A.G. Miller. The obedience experiments.
Contemporary Psychology
1988; 33 (2): 101-104
View details for DOI 10.1037/025376
- The problem of construal in social inference and social psychology A distinctive approach to psychological research: The influence of Stanley Schachter 1987: 118–150
-
PERSISTENCE OF INACCURATE BELIEFS ABOUT THE SELF - PERSEVERANCE EFFECTS IN THE CLASSROOM
JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
1986; 50 (3): 482-491
Abstract
The perseverance of erroneous self-assessments was examined among high school students. Subjects were first exposed to either highly effective or thoroughly useless filmed instruction, leading, respectively, to their consequent success or failure. No-discounting subjects received no assistance in recognizing the relative superiority or inferiority of their instruction. Discounting subjects, by contrast, were subsequently shown the opposite instructional film, highlighting the obvious differences in instructional quality. Subsequent measures revealed that all subjects recognized the effectiveness or ineffectiveness of their instruction, although this contrast was clearer for discounting subjects. Nevertheless, both discounting and no-discounting subjects continued to draw unwarranted inferences--in line with their initial outcomes--about their personal capacities, immediately afterward. Dissociated and disguised measures of academic preferences and perceptions completed weeks later produced even more dramatic results: The continuing impact of initial outcomes was generally greater for discounting than no-discounting subjects.
View details for Web of Science ID A1986C109200003
View details for PubMedID 3701590
-
THE HOSTILE MEDIA PHENOMENON - BIASED PERCEPTION AND PERCEPTIONS OF MEDIA BIAS IN COVERAGE OF THE BEIRUT MASSACRE
JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
1985; 49 (3): 577-585
Abstract
After viewing identical samples of major network television coverage of the Beirut massacre, both pro-Israeli and pro-Arab partisans rated these programs, and those responsible for them, as being biased against their side. This hostile media phenomenon appears to involve the operation of two separate mechanisms. First, partisans evaluated the fairness of the media's sample of facts and arguments differently: in light of their own divergent views about the objective merits of each side's case and their corresponding views about the nature of unbiased coverage. Second, partisans reported different perceptions and recollections about the program content itself; that is, each group reported more negative references to their side than positive ones, and each predicted that the coverage would sway nonpartisans in a hostile direction. Within both partisan groups, furthermore, greater knowledge of the crisis was associated with stronger perceptions of media bias. Charges of media bias, we concluded, may reflect more than self-serving attempts to secure preferential treatment. They may result from the operation of basic cognitive and perceptual mechanisms, mechanisms that should prove relevant to perceptions of fairness or objectivity in a wide range of mediation and negotiation contexts.
View details for Web of Science ID A1985AQJ6600001
View details for PubMedID 4045697
-
SELF-KNOWLEDGE AND SOCIAL INFERENCE .1. THE IMPACT OF COGNITIVE-AFFECTIVE AND BEHAVIORAL-DATA
JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
1984; 46 (2): 280-293
View details for Web of Science ID A1984SG19700004
-
PUBLIC-OPINION AND CAPITAL-PUNISHMENT - A CLOSE EXAMINATION OF THE VIEWS OF ABOLITIONISTS AND RETENTIONISTS
CRIME & DELINQUENCY
1983; 29 (1): 116-169
View details for Web of Science ID A1983RD15400005
-
Informal covariation assessment: Data-based vs. theory-based judgments.
Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases
Cambridge University Press. 1982: 211–230
View details for DOI 10.1017/CBO9780511809477.016
-
Shortcomings in the attribution process: On the origins and maintenance of erroneous social assessments
Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases
Cambridge University Press. 1982: 129–152
View details for DOI 10.1017/CBO9780511809477.010
-
Persistence of impression of personal persuasiveness: Persistence of erroneous self-perceptions outside the debriefing paradigm
Personalty and Social Psychology Bulletin
1981; 7: 257-263
View details for DOI 10.1177/014616728172012
- Social Cognitive Development: Frontiers and possible futures edited by Flavell, J., Ross, L. Cambridge Press. 1981
- The "intuitive scientist" formulation and its developmental implications Social cognitive development: Frontiers and possible futures edited by Flavell, J. H., Ross, L. Cambridge University Press. 1981: 1–42
-
PERSEVERANCE OF SOCIAL THEORIES - THE ROLE OF EXPLANATION IN THE PERSISTENCE OF DISCREDITED INFORMATION
JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
1980; 39 (6): 1037-1049
View details for Web of Science ID A1980KX30300004
- Human inference : strategies and shortcomings of social judgment Prentice-Hall. 1980
-
BIASED ASSIMILATION AND ATTITUDE POLARIZATION - EFFECTS OF PRIOR THEORIES ON SUBSEQUENTLY CONSIDERED EVIDENCE
JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
1979; 37 (11): 2098-2109
View details for Web of Science ID A1979HY30900017
- Afterthoughts on the intuitive psychologist Cognitive theories in social psychology edited by Berkowitz, L. Academic Press. 1979
-
SOCIAL EXPLANATION AND SOCIAL EXPECTATION - EFFECTS OF REAL AND HYPOTHETICAL EXPLANATIONS ON SUBJECTIVE LIKELIHOOD
JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
1977; 35 (11): 817-829
View details for Web of Science ID A1977EA92600005
- Empirical data and judicial decision-making: Capital punishment research Capital punishment edited by Bedau, H., Pierce, C. AMS Press. 1977
-
SOCIAL ROLES, SOCIAL-CONTROL, AND BIASES IN SOCIAL-PERCEPTION PROCESSES
JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
1977; 35 (7): 485-494
View details for Web of Science ID A1977DQ55400003
-
FALSE CONSENSUS EFFECT - EGOCENTRIC BIAS IN SOCIAL-PERCEPTION AND ATTRIBUTION PROCESSES
JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
1977; 13 (3): 279-301
View details for Web of Science ID A1977DD62100007
-
PROBLEMS IN INTERPRETATION OF SELF-SERVING ASYMMETRIES IN CAUSAL ATTRIBUTION - REPLY
SOCIOMETRY
1977; 40 (2): 112-114
View details for Web of Science ID A1977DN34700002
-
THE INTUITIVE PSYCHOLOGIST AND HIS SHORTCOMINGS DISTORTIONS IN THE ATTRIBUTION PROCESS
ADVANCES IN EXPERIMENTAL SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
Academic Press. 1977: 173–220
View details for DOI 10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60357-3
-
ROLE OF ATTRIBUTION PROCESSES IN CONFORMITY AND DISSENT - REVISITING ASCH SITUATION
AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGIST
1976; 31 (2): 148-157
View details for Web of Science ID A1976BF07100008
-
INTIMACY IN RESPONSE TO DIRECT GAZE
JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
1975; 11 (6): 592-613
View details for Web of Science ID A1975BB15700010
-
PERSEVERANCE IN SELF-PERCEPTION AND SOCIAL PERCEPTION - BIASED ATTRIBUTIONAL PROCESSES IN DEBRIEFING PARADIGM
JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
1975; 32 (5): 880-892
Abstract
Two experiments demonstrated that self-perceptions and social perceptions may persevere after the initial basis for such perceptions has been completely discredited. In both studies subjects first received false feedback, indicating that they had either succeeded or failed on a novel discrimination task and then were thoroughly debriefed concerning the predetermined and random nature of this outcome manipulation. In experiment 2, both the initial outcome manipulation and subsequent debriefing were watched and overheard by observers. Both actors and observers showed substantial perseverance of initial impressions concerning the actors' performance and abilities following a standard "outcome" debriefing. "Process" debriefing, in which explicit discussion of the perseverance process was provided, generally proved sufficient to eliminate erroneous self-perceptions. Biased attribution processes that might underlie perserverance phenomena and the implications of the present data for the ethical conduct of deception research are discussed.
View details for Web of Science ID A1975AW34500014
View details for PubMedID 1185517
-
Attribution of education outcome by professional and nonprofessional instructors.
Journal of personality and social psychology
1974; 29 (5): 609-618
View details for PubMedID 4833425
-
ATTRIBUTION OF EDUCATIONAL OUTCOMES BY PROFESSIONAL AND NONPROFESSIONAL INSTRUCTORS
JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
1974; 29 (5): 609-618
View details for Web of Science ID A1974S919200003
-
EFFECTS OF MISATTRIBUTION OF AROUSAL UPON ACQUISITION AND EXTINCTION OF A CONDITIONED EMOTIONAL RESPONSE
JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
1974; 30 (5): 673-682
View details for Web of Science ID A1974U611600010
View details for PubMedID 4443888
-
RETROSPECTIVE MISATTRIBUTION OF A CONDITIONED EMOTIONAL RESPONSE
JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
1974; 30 (5): 683-687
View details for Web of Science ID A1974U611600011
View details for PubMedID 4443889
-
SOME DIMENSIONS OF INTERNAL-EXTERNAL METAPHOR IN THEORIES OF PERSONALITY
JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY
1973; 41 (4): 471-492
View details for Web of Science ID A1973R604600001
View details for PubMedID 4761388
-
Dissonance, self-perception, and the perspective of others: A study in Cognitive cognitive dissonance
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology
1970; 6 (3): 304–315
View details for DOI 10.1016/0022-1031(70)90065-X
-
DISSONANCE, SELF-PERCEPTION, AND PERCEPTION OF OTHERS - A STUDY IN COGNITIVE COGNITIVE DISSONANCE
JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
1970; 6 (3): 304-?
View details for Web of Science ID A1970H002400005
-
COMMENTS ON EARNINGS-PRICE NOTE
STANFORD LAW REVIEW
1969; 21 (FEB): 644-649
View details for Web of Science ID A1969Y702300007
-
TOWARD AN ATTRIBUTION THERAPY - REDUCTION OF FEAR THROUGH INDUCED COGNITIVE-EMOTIONAL MISATTRIBUTION
JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
1969; 12 (4): 279-?
View details for Web of Science ID A1969D956000002
View details for PubMedID 5821854
-
ANTICIPATED EFFORT AND SUBJECTIVE PROBABILITY
JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
1966; 4 (1): 57-?
View details for Web of Science ID A19668030600008
View details for PubMedID 5965192