Ewart Thomas
Professor of Psychology, Emeritus
Administrative Appointments
-
Ralph & Claire Landau Visiting Professor of Psychology, The Chinese University of Hong Kong (2008 - 2008)
-
Visiting Professor of Mathematics, University of the West Indies (1995 - 1995)
-
Dean, School of Humanities & Sciences, Stanford University (1988 - 1993)
-
Associate Dean, School of Humanities & Sciences, Stanford University (1986 - 1988)
-
Chairman, Department of Psychology, Stanford University (1983 - 1986)
-
Visiting Professor of Mathematics, University of Guyana (1977 - 1977)
-
Professor of Psychology, Stanford University (1975 - Present)
-
Associate Professor of Psychology, Stanford University (1973 - 1975)
-
Visiting Associate Professor of Psychology, Stanford University (1972 - 1973)
-
Associate Professor of Psychology, University of Michigan (1971 - 1972)
-
Assistant Professor of Psychology, University of Michigan (1970 - 1971)
-
Honorary Research Assistant, Department of Psychology, University College London (1967 - 1970)
Honors & Awards
-
Fellow, John Simon Guggenheim Foundation (1976-1977)
-
Fellow, Center for Advanced Study in the Behavioral Sciences, Stanford, California (1979-1980)
-
University Fellow, Stanford University (1982-1984)
-
Distinguished Teaching Award, Associated Students of Stanford University (ASSU) (2002)
-
Bass University Fellow in Undergraduate Education, Stanford University (2013)
-
Dean's Award for Distinguished Teaching, School of Humanities & Sciences (2016)
Boards, Advisory Committees, Professional Organizations
-
Chair, Ad Hoc Committee of the Academic Senate on Reserve Officer Training Corps programs, Stanford University (2010 - 2011)
-
Member, Judicial Affairs Panel (2005 - 2007)
-
Member of the Vice-Chancellor’s Ad Hoc Committee to investigate ethnic and gender discrimination, St. Augustine campus, University of the West Indies (2005 - 2005)
-
Chair, Committee on Undergraduate Standards & Policy of the Academic Senate, Stanford University (2002 - 2004)
-
Chair, Committee on Committees of the Academic Senate, Stanford University (1998 - 2000)
-
Member of the Academic Senate, Stanford University (1998 - 2000)
-
Member of the Selection Committee, Alan T. Waterman Award, National Academy of Sciences (1994 - 1996)
-
Member of the Advisory Committee, Office of Science and Engineering Personnel, National Research Council (1993 - 1998)
-
Member of the Board on Testing and Assessment, National Research Council (1993 - 1997)
-
Member of the Chancellor's Review Commission, University of Guyana (1991 - 1991)
-
Member of Editorial Board, Psychological Bulletin (1987 - 1989)
-
Consulting Editor, Psychological Review (1985 - 1990)
-
Member of various Fellowship and Review Committees, National Institute of Mental Health (1972 - Present)
-
Member of various Fellowship and Review Committees, National Science Foundation (1972 - Present)
-
Member of various Fellowship and Review Committees, National Institute of Education (1972 - Present)
Professional Education
-
L.L.D. (Hon.), University of the West Indies, Jamaica (1989)
-
Ph.D., University of Cambridge, England, Statistics (1967)
-
B.Sc., University of the West Indies, Jamaica, Mathematics (1963)
Current Research and Scholarly Interests
Theoretical and experimental analyses of information processing, equity, and of small-group processes; statistical methods.
All Publications
-
Neighborhood effects on use of African-American Vernacular English.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
2015; 112 (38): 11817-11822
Abstract
African-American Vernacular English (AAVE) is systematic, rooted in history, and important as an identity marker and expressive resource for its speakers. In these respects, it resembles other vernacular or nonstandard varieties, like Cockney or Appalachian English. But like them, AAVE can trigger discrimination in the workplace, housing market, and schools. Understanding what shapes the relative use of AAVE vs. Standard American English (SAE) is important for policy and scientific reasons. This work presents, to our knowledge, the first experimental estimates of the effects of moving into lower-poverty neighborhoods on AAVE use. We use data on non-Hispanic African-American youth (n = 629) from a large-scale, randomized residential mobility experiment called Moving to Opportunity (MTO), which enrolled a sample of mostly minority families originally living in distressed public housing. Audio recordings of the youth were transcribed and coded for the use of five grammatical and five phonological AAVE features to construct a measure of the proportion of possible instances, or tokens, in which speakers use AAVE rather than SAE speech features. Random assignment to receive a housing voucher to move into a lower-poverty area (the intention-to-treat effect) led youth to live in neighborhoods (census tracts) with an 11 percentage point lower poverty rate on average over the next 10-15 y and reduced the share of AAVE tokens by ∼3 percentage points compared with the MTO control group youth. The MTO effect on AAVE use equals approximately half of the difference in AAVE frequency observed between youth whose parents have a high school diploma and those whose parents do not.
View details for DOI 10.1073/pnas.1500176112
View details for PubMedID 26351663
View details for PubMedCentralID PMC4586846
-
Neighborhood effects on use of African-American Vernacular English
PROCEEDINGS OF THE NATIONAL ACADEMY OF SCIENCES OF THE UNITED STATES OF AMERICA
2015; 112 (38): 11817-11822
View details for DOI 10.1073/pnas.1500176112
View details for Web of Science ID 000361525100041
-
A Little CFTR Goes a Long Way: CFTR-Dependent Sweat Secretion from G551D and R117H-5T Cystic Fibrosis Subjects Taking Ivacaftor
PLOS ONE
2014; 9 (2)
Abstract
To determine if oral dosing with the CFTR-potentiator ivacaftor (VX-770, Kalydeco) improves CFTR-dependent sweating in CF subjects carrying G551D or R117H-5T mutations, we optically measured sweat secretion from 32-143 individually identified glands in each of 8 CF subjects; 6 F508del/G551D, one G551D/R117H-5T, and one I507del/R117H-5T. Two subjects were tested only (-) ivacaftor, 3 only (+) ivacaftor and 3 (+/-) ivacaftor (1-5 tests per condition). The total number of gland measurements was 852 (-) ivacaftor and 906 (+) ivacaftor. A healthy control was tested 4 times (51 glands). For each gland we measured both CFTR-independent (M-sweat) and CFTR-dependent (C-sweat); C-sweat was stimulated with a β-adrenergic cocktail that elevated [cAMP]i while blocking muscarinic receptors. Absent ivacaftor, almost all CF glands produced M-sweat on all tests, but only 1/593 glands produced C-sweat (10 tests, 5 subjects). By contrast, 6/6 subjects (113/342 glands) produced C-sweat in the (+) ivacaftor condition, but with large inter-subject differences; 3-74% of glands responded with C/M sweat ratios 0.04%-2.57% of the average WT ratio of 0.265. Sweat volume losses cause proportionally larger underestimates of CFTR function at lower sweat rates. The losses were reduced by measuring C/M ratios in 12 glands from each subject that had the highest M-sweat rates. Remaining losses were estimated from single channel data and used to correct the C/M ratios, giving estimates of CFTR function (+) ivacaftor = 1.6%-7.7% of the WT average. These estimates are in accord with single channel data and transcript analysis, and suggest that significant clinical benefit can be produced by low levels of CFTR function.
View details for DOI 10.1371/journal.pone.0088564
View details for Web of Science ID 000331254600090
View details for PubMedID 24520399
View details for PubMedCentralID PMC3919757
-
Choosing a physician depends on how you want to feel: The role of ideal affect in health-related decision making.
Emotion
2014; 14 (1): 187-192
Abstract
When given a choice, how do people decide which physician to select? Although significant research has demonstrated that how people actually feel (their "actual affect") influences their health care preferences, how people ideally want to feel (their "ideal affect") may play an even greater role. Specifically, we predicted that people trust physicians whose affective characteristics match their ideal affect, which leads people to prefer those physicians more. Consistent with this prediction, the more participants wanted to feel high arousal positive states on average (ideal HAP; e.g., excited), the more likely they were to select a HAP-focused physician. Similarly, the more people wanted to feel low arousal positive states on average (ideal LAP; e.g., calm), the more likely they were to select a LAP-focused physician. Also as predicted, these links were mediated by perceived physician trustworthiness. Notably, while participants' ideal affect predicted physician preference, actual affect (how much people actually felt HAP and LAP on average) did not. These findings suggest that people base serious decisions on how they want to feel, and highlight the importance of considering ideal affect in models of decision making preferences.
View details for DOI 10.1037/a0034372
View details for PubMedID 24188062
-
A little CFTR goes a long way: CFTR-dependent sweat secretion from G551D and R117H-5T cystic fibrosis subjects taking ivacaftor.
PloS one
2014; 9 (2)
Abstract
To determine if oral dosing with the CFTR-potentiator ivacaftor (VX-770, Kalydeco) improves CFTR-dependent sweating in CF subjects carrying G551D or R117H-5T mutations, we optically measured sweat secretion from 32-143 individually identified glands in each of 8 CF subjects; 6 F508del/G551D, one G551D/R117H-5T, and one I507del/R117H-5T. Two subjects were tested only (-) ivacaftor, 3 only (+) ivacaftor and 3 (+/-) ivacaftor (1-5 tests per condition). The total number of gland measurements was 852 (-) ivacaftor and 906 (+) ivacaftor. A healthy control was tested 4 times (51 glands). For each gland we measured both CFTR-independent (M-sweat) and CFTR-dependent (C-sweat); C-sweat was stimulated with a β-adrenergic cocktail that elevated [cAMP]i while blocking muscarinic receptors. Absent ivacaftor, almost all CF glands produced M-sweat on all tests, but only 1/593 glands produced C-sweat (10 tests, 5 subjects). By contrast, 6/6 subjects (113/342 glands) produced C-sweat in the (+) ivacaftor condition, but with large inter-subject differences; 3-74% of glands responded with C/M sweat ratios 0.04%-2.57% of the average WT ratio of 0.265. Sweat volume losses cause proportionally larger underestimates of CFTR function at lower sweat rates. The losses were reduced by measuring C/M ratios in 12 glands from each subject that had the highest M-sweat rates. Remaining losses were estimated from single channel data and used to correct the C/M ratios, giving estimates of CFTR function (+) ivacaftor = 1.6%-7.7% of the WT average. These estimates are in accord with single channel data and transcript analysis, and suggest that significant clinical benefit can be produced by low levels of CFTR function.
View details for DOI 10.1371/journal.pone.0088564
View details for PubMedID 24520399
-
In Vivo Readout of CFTR Function: Ratiometric Measurement of CFTR-Dependent Secretion by Individual, Identifiable Human Sweat Glands
PLOS ONE
2013; 8 (10)
Abstract
To assess CFTR function in vivo, we developed a bioassay that monitors and compares CFTR-dependent and CFTR-independent sweat secretion in parallel for multiple (∼50) individual, identified glands in each subject. Sweating was stimulated by intradermally injected agonists and quantified by optically measuring spherical sweat bubbles in an oil-layer that contained dispersed, water soluble dye particles that partitioned into the sweat bubbles, making them highly visible. CFTR-independent secretion (M-sweat) was stimulated with methacholine, which binds to muscarinic receptors and elevates cytosolic calcium. CFTR-dependent secretion (C-sweat) was stimulated with a β-adrenergic cocktail that elevates cytosolic cAMP while blocking muscarinic receptors. A C-sweat/M-sweat ratio was determined on a gland-by-gland basis to compensate for differences unrelated to CFTR function, such as gland size. The average ratio provides an approximately linear readout of CFTR function: the heterozygote ratio is ∼0.5 the control ratio and for CF subjects the ratio is zero. During assay development, we measured C/M ratios in 6 healthy controls, 4 CF heterozygotes, 18 CF subjects and 4 subjects with 'CFTR-related' conditions. The assay discriminated all groups clearly. It also revealed consistent differences in the C/M ratio among subjects within groups. We hypothesize that these differences reflect, at least in part, levels of CFTR expression, which are known to vary widely. When C-sweat rates become very low the C/M ratio also tended to decrease; we hypothesize that this nonlinearity reflects ductal fluid absorption. We also discovered that M-sweating potentiates the subsequent C-sweat response. We then used potentiation as a surrogate for drugs that can increase CFTR-dependent secretion. This bioassay provides an additional method for assessing CFTR function in vivo, and is well suited for within-subject tests of systemic, CFTR-directed therapeutics.
View details for DOI 10.1371/journal.pone.0077114
View details for Web of Science ID 000326152300015
View details for PubMedID 24204751
View details for PubMedCentralID PMC3811985
-
In vivo readout of CFTR function: ratiometric measurement of CFTR-dependent secretion by individual, identifiable human sweat glands.
PloS one
2013; 8 (10)
Abstract
To assess CFTR function in vivo, we developed a bioassay that monitors and compares CFTR-dependent and CFTR-independent sweat secretion in parallel for multiple (∼50) individual, identified glands in each subject. Sweating was stimulated by intradermally injected agonists and quantified by optically measuring spherical sweat bubbles in an oil-layer that contained dispersed, water soluble dye particles that partitioned into the sweat bubbles, making them highly visible. CFTR-independent secretion (M-sweat) was stimulated with methacholine, which binds to muscarinic receptors and elevates cytosolic calcium. CFTR-dependent secretion (C-sweat) was stimulated with a β-adrenergic cocktail that elevates cytosolic cAMP while blocking muscarinic receptors. A C-sweat/M-sweat ratio was determined on a gland-by-gland basis to compensate for differences unrelated to CFTR function, such as gland size. The average ratio provides an approximately linear readout of CFTR function: the heterozygote ratio is ∼0.5 the control ratio and for CF subjects the ratio is zero. During assay development, we measured C/M ratios in 6 healthy controls, 4 CF heterozygotes, 18 CF subjects and 4 subjects with 'CFTR-related' conditions. The assay discriminated all groups clearly. It also revealed consistent differences in the C/M ratio among subjects within groups. We hypothesize that these differences reflect, at least in part, levels of CFTR expression, which are known to vary widely. When C-sweat rates become very low the C/M ratio also tended to decrease; we hypothesize that this nonlinearity reflects ductal fluid absorption. We also discovered that M-sweating potentiates the subsequent C-sweat response. We then used potentiation as a surrogate for drugs that can increase CFTR-dependent secretion. This bioassay provides an additional method for assessing CFTR function in vivo, and is well suited for within-subject tests of systemic, CFTR-directed therapeutics.
View details for DOI 10.1371/journal.pone.0077114
View details for PubMedID 24204751
-
CHOLINERGIC POTENTIATION OF CFTR-DEPENDENT SECRETION BY INDIVIDUAL, IDENTIFIABLE SWEAT GLANDS
WILEY-BLACKWELL. 2012: 299–299
View details for Web of Science ID 000308882000304
-
Comparing the Eyes Depicted in Japanese Portraits of Beautiful Women: The Meiji and Modern Periods
AESTHETIC PLASTIC SURGERY
2012; 36 (3): 504-510
Abstract
The women portrayed in the bijin-ga of the past, particularly those from the Meiji Period (1868–1912), tended not to show much resemblance to those of women portrayed in the more modern bijin-ga (from after World War II), and such an observation came across as a possible indication that Japanese standards of beauty have changed over the two eras. To examine whether the apparent discrepancy can be interpreted as an actual change in the standards or not, a study was designed with the aim of assigning numeric values to several aspects of the eyes and testing for the presence of a statistically significant difference in each of the aspects between the Meiji bijin-ga and the modern bijin-ga.For this study, 29 Meiji bijin-ga and 36 modern bijin-ga were selected. The eye was chosen as the subject of comparison, and five aspects were categorized and measured: (1) presence or absence of a double fold, (2) eye width, (3) eye height, (4) eyebrow-to-upper lid distance, and (D) corneal diameter. The eye width, the eye height, and the eyebrow-to-upper lid distance were divided by the corneal diameter to derive standardized grounds for comparison.The difference in double-fold frequencies between the Meiji bijin-ga (24%) and the modern bijin-ga (36%) was not found to be statistically significant (p=0.298). There was no difference in the eye width-to-corneal diameter ratio between the Meiji bijin-ga (mean 2.57±0.6) and the modern bijin-ga (mean 2.61±0.85) (p=0.86). The eye height-to-corneal diameter ratio derived from the Meiji bijin-ga (mean 0.62±0.15) was significantly smaller than that derived from the modern bijin-ga (mean 0.82±0.18) (p=0.000). The eyebrow to upper lid distance-to-corneal diameter ratio derived from the Meiji bijin-ga (mean 2.21±0.83) was significantly greater than that derived from the modern bijin-ga (mean 1.36±0.78) (p=0.000).The results of the study support the notion that Westernization contributed to bringing about changes in the Japanese standards of beautiful eyes in the context of bijin-ga. However, the fact that the changeover has not occurred in all the categories in question does not indicate that the Occidental characteristics came to be emulated in their entirety.This journal requires that authors assign a level of evidence to each article. For a full description of these Evidence-Based Medicine ratings, please refer to the Table of Contents or the online Instructions to Authors at http://www.springer.com/00266.
View details for DOI 10.1007/s00266-011-9857-y
View details for PubMedID 22302188
- Studying Teacher Effectiveness: The Challenges of Developing Valid Measures The SAGE Handbook of Measurement edited by Walford, G., Tucker, E. Sage Publications Ltd. 2010
- Value-Added Modeling of Teacher Effectiveness: An Exploration of Stability across Models and Contexts Educational Policy Analysis Archives 2010; 18 (23)
-
YOU'RE AS YOUNG AS YOU WANT TO FEEL: CULTURAL VARIATION IN IDEAL AFFECT ACROSS THE LIFESPAN
OXFORD UNIV PRESS INC. 2009: 237–237
View details for Web of Science ID 000271794100153
- Mathematical and Empirical Studies of English/Creole Language Variation Proceedings of the 16th Caribbean Academy of Sciences Biennial Conference on Science and Technology: Vehicles for Sustainable Economic Development in the Caribbean Grenada, West Indies.. 2008: 1–19; 11–13
-
Group differences in fairness perceptions and decision making in voting rights cases
LAW AND HUMAN BEHAVIOR
2006; 30 (5): 543-560
Abstract
Participants recruited from one Historically Black University (HBU) and two predominantly White higher-education institutions evaluated and decided simulated voting rights case summaries in which the plaintiff was either a racially-defined (African American) or a nonracially-defined (farmers) minority group. Contrary to social identity and social justice findings of an in-group bias, the present study showed greater support at all institutions for the voting rights of the African Americans than for the rural farmers, and the greatest support for both minority groups was found at the HBU. Perceived evidence strength was a better predictor of decisions than perceived unfairness, and both of these predictor variables completely mediated the effects of institution-type and involvement of a racially-defined group on decisions.
View details for DOI 10.1007/s10979-006-9046-8
View details for Web of Science ID 000241560400001
View details for PubMedID 17019616
- Congener Development in Jamaican Rum over an Aging Period of Three Years Journal of Science and Technology 1995; 6: 30-51
-
DISTRIBUTIONAL ASSUMPTIONS AND OBSERVED CONSERVATISM IN THE THEORY OF SIGNAL DETECTABILITY
JOURNAL OF MATHEMATICAL PSYCHOLOGY
1991; 35 (4): 443-470
View details for Web of Science ID A1991GU75800002
-
BEHAVIOR-DEPENDENT CONTEXTS FOR REPEATED PLAYS OF THE PRISONERS-DILEMMA
JOURNAL OF CONFLICT RESOLUTION
1988; 32 (4): 699-726
View details for Web of Science ID A1988R714500005
-
ON THE ROLE OF PATCH DENSITY AND PATCH VARIABILITY IN CENTRAL-PLACE FORAGING
THEORETICAL POPULATION BIOLOGY
1988; 34 (3): 266-278
View details for Web of Science ID A1988R629900003
-
LIABILITY AS A FUNCTION OF PLAINTIFF AND DEFENDANT FAULT
JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
1987; 53 (5): 843-857
View details for Web of Science ID A1987K850100003
-
BEHAVIOR-DEPENDENT CONTEXTS FOR REPEATED PLAYS OF THE PRISONERS-DILEMMA .2. DYNAMIC ASPECTS OF THE EVOLUTION OF COOPERATION
JOURNAL OF THEORETICAL BIOLOGY
1987; 128 (3): 297-315
Abstract
Iterated Prisoner's Dilemma models are proposed in which, at any trial, the probability of staying in the game depends on the outcome of the previous trial. If a player's choice depends on its own play (cooperate or defect) at the previous trial, it becomes possible for cooperative strategies to increase when rare in a population of egoists. A dynamic analysis is used to demonstrate that stable polymorphisms may result, and may involve more strategies than just Tit-for-Tat and all-Defect. The tendency for clustering among like strategists to enhance their initial increase when rare is also explored dynamically.
View details for PubMedID 3444341
-
ON CALCULATING OPTIMAL CONTRIBUTORY NEGLIGENCE RULES
SOCIAL CHOICE AND WELFARE
1985; 2 (1): 65-85
View details for Web of Science ID A1985AXL4800006
-
ANTICIPATORY AND FANTASTIC ROLE ENACTMENT IN PRESCHOOL TRIADS
DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY
1984; 20 (5): 807-814
View details for Web of Science ID A1984TH40200007
-
THE SHORT-TERM DYNAMICS OF SOCIAL-ORGANIZATION IN PRESCHOOL TRIADS
CHILD DEVELOPMENT
1984; 55 (3): 1051-1070
Abstract
Sequential dependencies in solitary and interactive states of social organization were examined as a function of age, sex, and type of toy in 12 triads of 3 1/2- and 5-year-old children. Each triad was observed during 2 30-min sessions, one in which objects with highly specific functions (e.g., dolls, trucks) were available, and one in which objects with relatively ambiguous functions (e.g., pipe cleaners, cardboard cylinders) were present. The results indicated that, while old and young triads did not differ in the tendency to initiate triadic interaction when the preceding state was solitary, old triads were more likely than young triads to maintain the triadic state and to shift to that state from the dyadic state. Boy triads were more likely than girl triads to remain in a solitary state and less likely to shift to and remain in a dyadic state, though no sex difference was found in the transition probabilities when the preceding state was triadic. Triads were more likely to remain in a solitary state in the presence of high-specificity toys than in the presence of low-specificity toys. Verbal metacommunication was more frequent among old triads than young triads, and it facilitated maintenance, but not initiation, of interactive states. The distinction between initiation and maintenance tendencies was formalized in a Markov model of the dynamics of social organization, and the parameter estimates yielded by the model were used to provide a simplified description of the "main" effects of age, sex, and type of toy. It is argued that models such as this one are useful in circumventing certain restrictions on the possible interpretations of raw sequential statistics.
View details for Web of Science ID A1984SW27900037
View details for PubMedID 6734306
-
NOTES ON EFFORT AND ACHIEVEMENT-ORIENTED BEHAVIOR
PSYCHOLOGICAL REVIEW
1983; 90 (1): 1-20
View details for Web of Science ID A1983QF23100001
View details for PubMedID 6844476
-
THE INFLUENCE OF OWN AND OTHER OUTCOME ON SATISFACTION AND CHOICE OF TASK-DIFFICULTY
ORGANIZATIONAL BEHAVIOR AND HUMAN PERFORMANCE
1983; 32 (3): 399-416
View details for Web of Science ID A1983RT05000005
- Effort and optimality in the new Soviet incentive model Economics of Planning 1981; 17: 23-36
-
PROCESSING INSTRUCTIONS, MARKEDNESS, AND CONGRUITY EFFECTS IN A SENTENCE VERIFICATION TASK
JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY-HUMAN PERCEPTION AND PERFORMANCE
1981; 7 (3): 701-718
View details for Web of Science ID A1981LV21200022
-
ON APPROPRIATE PROCEDURES FOR COMBINING PROBABILITY-DISTRIBUTIONS WITHIN THE SAME FAMILY
JOURNAL OF MATHEMATICAL PSYCHOLOGY
1980; 21 (2): 136-152
View details for Web of Science ID A1980KA70700003
- Bias and discriminability in group performance Research Directions of Black Psychologists edited by Boykin, A. W., Fraklin, A. J., Yates, J. F. New York: Russell Sage Foundation. 1979
-
DYNAMICS OF 2-PERSON INTERACTIONS
PSYCHOLOGICAL REVIEW
1979; 86 (4): 331-360
View details for Web of Science ID A1979HH28500002
- Interdependence between the processing of temporal and non-temporal information Attention and Performance VII edited by Requin, J. Hillsdale: Erlbaum. 1979
- An analysis of mother-child interaction Research Directions of Black Psychologists. edited by Boykin, A. W., Franklin, A. J., Yates, J. F. New York: Russell Sage Foundation. 1979
-
EFFECT OF LEVEL OF CONFUSABILITY ON REPORTING LETTERS FROM BRIEFLY PRESENTED VISUAL-DISPLAYS
PERCEPTION & PSYCHOPHYSICS
1977; 21 (3): 269-279
View details for Web of Science ID A1977CZ92500009
-
CONTROL OF ATTENTION IN PROCESSING OF TEMPORAL AND SPATIAL INFORMATION IN COMPLEX VISUAL-PATTERNS
JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY-HUMAN PERCEPTION AND PERFORMANCE
1977; 3 (2): 243-250
Abstract
Eighteen stimuli were created by orthogonally varying the area (A), perimeter (P), and exposure duration of checkerboard patterns. Subjects judged either the area and duration of the presented shapes (area-time group) or the perimeter and duration (perimeter-time group) of the same figures. Perceived duration, area, and perimeter varied with changes in the uninstructed an the instructed stimulus dimensions. The discussion focuses on the ability of subjects to control the allocation of attention to instructed stimulus dimensions, as well as on the evidence in the data in favor of detailed sequential or fast holistic processing. It is suggested that the concept of stimulus complexity, defined as P2/A, is useful in describing the temporal and spatial illusions obtained in this study.
View details for Web of Science ID A1977DD33200007
View details for PubMedID 864396
-
SIMULTANEOUS TIME AND SIZE PERCEPTION
PERCEPTION & PSYCHOPHYSICS
1976; 19 (4): 353-360
View details for Web of Science ID A1976BR22200013
-
APPARENT WEIGHT OF EVIDENCE, DECISION CRITERIA, AND CONFIDENCE RATINGS IN JUROR DECISION-MAKING
PSYCHOLOGICAL REVIEW
1976; 83 (6): 442-465
View details for Web of Science ID A1976CM14600003
-
ANALYSES OF PARENT-INFANT INTERACTION
PSYCHOLOGICAL REVIEW
1976; 83 (2): 141-156
View details for Web of Science ID A1976BJ54400003
-
FACILITY LOCATION WITH CHANGING DEMAND LEVELS
GEOGRAPHICAL ANALYSIS
1976; 8 (4): 376-394
View details for Web of Science ID A1976CK37800003
-
VISUAL MASKING EFFECTS ON DURATION, SIZE, AND FORM DISCRIMINATION
PERCEPTION & PSYCHOPHYSICS
1976; 19 (4): 321-327
View details for Web of Science ID A1976BR22200007
-
EXTRACTING IDENTITY AND LOCATION INFORMATION FROM BRIEFLY PRESENTED LETTER ARRAYS
PERCEPTION & PSYCHOPHYSICS
1976; 20 (4): 243-258
View details for Web of Science ID A1976CM14700004
-
TECHNOLOGICAL IMPLICATIONS OF WAGE-PROFIT FRONTIER
JOURNAL OF ECONOMIC THEORY
1975; 11 (2): 263-282
View details for Web of Science ID A1975AV04300005
-
CRITERION ADJUSTMENT AND PROBABILITY MATCHING
PERCEPTION & PSYCHOPHYSICS
1975; 18 (2): 158-162
View details for Web of Science ID A1975AN95000017
-
SPATIAL DEMAND MODELS WITH UNIFORM RATE OF GROWTH
ENVIRONMENT AND PLANNING A
1975; 7 (5): 589-599
View details for Web of Science ID A1975AT66000008
-
DUALITY OF SIMULTANEOUS TIME AND SIZE PERCEPTION
PERCEPTION & PSYCHOPHYSICS
1975; 18 (1): 44-48
View details for Web of Science ID A1975AK22400008
-
NOTE ON SEQUENTIAL PROBABILITY RATIO TEST
PSYCHOMETRIKA
1975; 40 (1): 107-111
View details for Web of Science ID A1975AC39100006
-
COGNITIVE PROCESSING AND TIME PERCEPTION
PERCEPTION & PSYCHOPHYSICS
1975; 17 (4): 363-367
View details for Web of Science ID A1975AA99600006
-
TIME PERCEPTION AND FILLED-DURATION ILLUSION
PERCEPTION & PSYCHOPHYSICS
1974; 16 (3): 449-458
View details for Web of Science ID A1974V088000008
-
SELECTIVITY OF PREPARATION
PSYCHOLOGICAL REVIEW
1974; 81 (5): 442-464
View details for Web of Science ID A1974U172200004
- An analysis of the effects of group discussion on individual risk preferences Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1973; 25: 192-198
- On expectancy and the speed and accuracy of responses Attention and Performance IV edited by Komblum, S. New York: Academic Press. 1973: 613–626
-
CLASS OF ADDITIVE LEARNING MODELS - ERROR-CORRECTING AND PROBABILITY MATCHING
JOURNAL OF MATHEMATICAL PSYCHOLOGY
1973; 10 (3): 241-264
View details for Web of Science ID A1973Q636400001
-
IMPLICATIONS OF LATENCY DATA FOR THRESHOLD AND NONTHRESHOLD MODELS OF SIGNAL DETECTION
JOURNAL OF MATHEMATICAL PSYCHOLOGY
1972; 9 (3): 253-?
View details for Web of Science ID A1972N095400001
-
SUFFICIENT CONDITIONS FOR MONOTONE HAZARD RATE AN APPLICATION TO LATENCY-PROBABILITY CURVES
JOURNAL OF MATHEMATICAL PSYCHOLOGY
1971; 8 (3): 303-?
View details for Web of Science ID A1971K179800001
-
A MODEL FOR SUBJECTIVE GROUPING IN TYPEWRITING
QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY
1970; 22: 353-?
View details for Web of Science ID A1970H473000002
-
PROBABILITY MATCHING AS A BASIS FOR DETECTION AND RECOGNITION DECISIONS
PSYCHOLOGICAL REVIEW
1970; 77 (1): 65-72
View details for Web of Science ID A1970Y304900005
-
PREPARATION IN SIMPLE REACTION TASKS
BRITISH PSYCHOLOGICAL SOC. 1970: 241–42
View details for Web of Science ID A1970Y372500045
-
ON EXPECTANCY AND AVERAGE REACTION TIME
BRITISH JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY
1970; 61 (1): 33-?
View details for Web of Science ID A1970F625400005
-
DISTRIBUTION FREE TESTS FOR MIXED PROBABILITY DISTRIBUTIONS
BIOMETRIKA
1969; 56 (3): 475-?
View details for Web of Science ID A1969E794300002
-
ALTERNATIVE MODELS FOR INFORMATION PROCESSING-CONSTRUCTING NON-PARAMETRIC TESTS
BRITISH JOURNAL OF MATHEMATICAL & STATISTICAL PSYCHOLOGY
1969; 22: 105-?
View details for Web of Science ID A1969G177700001
-
REACTION-TIME STUDIES - ANTICIPATION AND INTERACTION OF RESPONSES
BRITISH JOURNAL OF MATHEMATICAL & STATISTICAL PSYCHOLOGY
1967; 20: 1-?
View details for Web of Science ID A19679852100001
View details for PubMedID 6073862
-
AN OPTIMUM INTERVAL IN ASSESSMENT OF PAIN THRESHOLD
QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY
1967; 19: 54-?
View details for Web of Science ID A19678937900007
View details for PubMedID 6041683
-
ON DETERMINING PAIN THRESHOLDS USING LIMITING METHOD
QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY
1966; 18: 270-?
View details for Web of Science ID A19668190000011
View details for PubMedID 5947483
- Mathematical models for the clustered firing of single cortical neurons British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology 1966; 19: 151-162